Primate


An order of mammals














A primate (/ˈprmt/ (About this soundlisten) PRY-mayt) (from Latin primat-, from primus: "prime, first rank") is a mammalian animal constituting the taxonomic order Primates. Primates arose 55–85 million years ago from small terrestrial mammals (Primatomorpha), who adapted to living in the trees of tropical forests: many primate characteristics represent adaptations to life in this challenging environment, including large brains, visual acuity, color vision, altered shoulder girdle, and dexterous hands. Primates range in size from Madame Berthe's mouse lemur, which weighs 30 g (1 oz), to the eastern gorilla, weighing over 200 kg (440 lb). There are 190–448 species of living primates, depending on which classification is utilized. New primate species continue to be discovered: over 25 species were described in the first decade of the 2000's, and eleven since 2010.


Primates are divided into two distinct suborders (see diagram under History of terminology). The first is the strepsirrhines (from Greek "wet-nosed") - lemurs, galagos, and lorisids. (The colloquial names ending in "-nosed" actually refer to the rhinarium of the primate.) The second is haplorhines - the "dry-nosed" primates (from Greek "simple-nosed") - tarsiers, monkeys, and apes, the latter including humans. Simians (infraorder Simiiformes, from Greek simos "flat-nosed") are monkeys and apes, cladistically including: the catarrhines (from Latin "narrow nosed") consisting of the Old World monkeys and apes; and the platyrrhines (from Latin "flat-nosed"): this division occurred about 60 million years ago.. Forty million years ago, simians from Africa migrated to South America, presumably by drifting on debris, and gave rise to the New World monkeys. Twenty five million years ago the remaining Old World simians (catarrhines) split into apes and Old World monkeys. Common names for the simians are the (Old World) baboons, macaques, gibbons, and great apes; and the (New World) capuchins, howlers and squirrel monkeys.


Primates are characterized by large brains (relative to body size) compared to other mammals, as well as an increased reliance on visual acuity at the expense of the sense of smell, which is the dominant sensory system in most mammals. These features are more developed in monkeys and apes, and noticeably less so in lorises and lemurs. Trichromatism, three independent channels for conveying color information, is present in some primates. Except for apes, primates have tails. Most primates also have opposable thumbs. Many species are sexually dimorphic; differences may include muscle mass, fat distribution, pelvic width, canine tooth size, hair distribution, and coloration. Primates have slower rates of development than other similarly sized mammals, reach maturity later, and have longer lifespans. Depending on the species, adults may live in solitude, in mated pairs, or in groups of up to hundreds of members. Some primates, including some great apes (gorillas and humans) and baboons, are primarily terrestrial rather than arboreal; but all species possess adaptations for climbing trees. Arboreal locomotion techniques used include leaping from tree to tree, and swinging between branches of trees (brachiation); terresterial locomotion techniques include walking on two limbs (bipedalism) and modified walking on four limbs (knuckle-walking).


Primates are among the most social of animals, forming pairs or family groups, uni-male harems, and multi-male/multi-female groups. Non-human primates have at four types of social systems, many defined by the amount of movement by adolescent females occurring between groups. Most primate species remain at least partly arboreal: the exceptions are some great apes, baboons - and humans, who left the trees for the ground and now inhabit every continent.


Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create opportunities for the transmission of zoonotic diseases, especially diseases caused by viruses, including herpes, measles, ebola, rabies, and hepatitis. Thousands of non-human primates are used around the world in research because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction. Common threats include deforestation, forest fragmentation, monkey drives, and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing for agriculture most threatens primates.




Contents





  • 1 History of terminology


  • 2 Classification of living primates


  • 3 Phylogeny and genetics

    • 3.1 Evolution


    • 3.2 Hybrids


    • 3.3 Clones



  • 4 Anatomy and physiology

    • 4.1 Eyes


    • 4.2 Skull


    • 4.3 Hands


    • 4.4 Torso


    • 4.5 Face


    • 4.6 Color vision


    • 4.7 Sexual dimorphism


    • 4.8 Locomotion



  • 5 Behavior

    • 5.1 Social systems


    • 5.2 Interspecific associations


    • 5.3 Cognition and communication


    • 5.4 Aggression


    • 5.5 Life history


    • 5.6 Diet, feeding and hunting

      • 5.6.1 Hunting



    • 5.7 As prey


    • 5.8 Tool use and manufacture

      • 5.8.1 Tool use


      • 5.8.2 Tool manufacture




  • 6 Ecology


  • 7 Interactions between humans and other primates

    • 7.1 Disease transmission


    • 7.2 Legal and social status


    • 7.3 Role in scientific research


    • 7.4 Extinction threats



  • 8 See also


  • 9 Footnotes


  • 10 References

    • 10.1 Literature cited



  • 11 Further reading


  • 12 External links




History of terminology


The English name "primates" is derived from Old French or French primat, from a noun use of Latin primat-, from primus ("prime, first rank").[2] The name was given by Carl Linnaeus because he thought this the "highest" order of animals.[3] The relationships among the different groups of primates were not clearly understood until relatively recently, so the commonly used terms are somewhat confused. For example, "ape" has been used either as an alternative for "monkey" or for any tailless, relatively human-like primate.[4]


Sir Wilfrid Le Gros Clark was one of the primatologists who developed the idea of trends in primate evolution and the methodology of arranging the living members of an order into an "ascending series" leading to humans.[5] Commonly used names for groups of primates such as "prosimians", "monkeys", "lesser apes", and "great apes" reflect this methodology. According to our current understanding of the evolutionary history of the primates, several of these groups are paraphyletic: a paraphyletic group is one which does not include all the descendants of the group's common ancestor.[6]


In contrast with Clark's methodology, modern classifications typically identify (or name) only those groupings that are monophyletic; that is, such a named group includes all the descendants of the group's common ancestor.[7]


The cladogram below shows one possible classification sequence of the living primates:[8][9] groups that use common (traditional) names are shown on the right.


Primates
Temporal range: Paleocene–Present, 55–0 Ma

PreЄ

Є

O

S

D

C

P

T

J

K

Pg

N











Aye-ayeRing-tailed lemurCapuchin monkeySpider monkeyGibbonTarsierRed slender lorisLion tamarinHamadryas baboonCommon chimpanzeePrimates - some families.jpg
About this image


Some primate families, from top to bottom: Daubentoniidae, Tarsiidae, Lemuridae, Lorisidae, Cebidae, Callitrichidae, Atelidae, Cercopithecidae, Hylobatidae, Hominidae.

Scientific classification edit
Kingdom:
Animalia
Phylum:
Chordata
Class:
Mammalia
Mirorder:
Primatomorpha
Order:
Primates
Linnaeus, 1758[1]
Suborders, Infraorders, Parvorders, Superfamilies, and Families (16)

  • See classification

Range of Non-human Primates.png
Range of the nonhuman primates (green)



.mw-parser-output table.cladeborder-spacing:0;margin:0;font-size:100%;line-height:100%;border-collapse:separate;width:auto.mw-parser-output table.clade table.cladewidth:100%.mw-parser-output table.clade tdborder:0;padding:0;vertical-align:middle;text-align:center.mw-parser-output table.clade td.clade-labelwidth:0.8em;border:0;padding:0 0.2em;vertical-align:bottom;text-align:center.mw-parser-output table.clade td.clade-slabelborder:0;padding:0 0.2em;vertical-align:top;text-align:center.mw-parser-output table.clade td.clade-barvertical-align:middle;text-align:left;padding:0 0.5em.mw-parser-output table.clade td.clade-leafborder:0;padding:0;text-align:left;vertical-align:middle.mw-parser-output table.clade td.clade-leafRborder:0;padding:0;text-align:right



Primatomorpha








 Dermoptera Cynocephalus volans Brehm1883 (white background).jpg



 Primates 






 Haplorhini 






 Simiiformes 






 Catarrhini 






 Hominoidea 






 Hominidae 






 Homininae 






 Hominini 








humans (genus Homo) Bechuana of Distinction-1841 (white background).jpg  





chimpanzees (genus Pan)PanTroglodytesSmit (white background).jpg  




 Gorillini 






gorillas (genus Gorilla) Gorila de llanura occidental. Gorilla gorilla - Blanca Martí de Ahumada (white background).jpg  







orangutans (subfamily Ponginae) Simia satyrus - 1837 - Print - Iconographia Zoologica - Special Collections University of Amsterdam - White Background.jpg  






gibbons (family Hylobatidae) Le gibbon (white background).jpg  






Old World monkeys (superfamily Cercopithecoidea) Cynocephalus doguera - 1700-1880 - Print - Iconographia Zoologica - Special Collections University of Amsterdam - (white background).tiff  






New World monkeys (parvorder Platyrrhini) Die Säugthiere in Abbildungen nach der Natur, mit Beschreibungen (Plate 8) (white background).jpg  




 Tarsiiformes 






tarsiers (superfamily Tarsioidea) Säugethiere vom Celebes- und Philippinen-Archipel (Taf. III) (white background) (1).jpg  





 Strepsirrhini 





Lemuriformes[a] 








lemurs (superfamily Lemuroidea) FMIB 46849 Primates Maki Moccoe Lemur catta (white background).jpeg  





lorises and allies (superfamily Lorisoidea) Nycticebus (white background).jpg  









prosimians

monkeys

great apes

humans

lesser apes


All groups with scientific names are monophyletic (that is, they are clades), and the sequence of scientific classification reflects the evolutionary history of the related lineages. Groups that are traditionally named are shown on the right; they form an "ascending series" (per Clark, see above), and several groups are paraphyletic:


  • "prosimians" contain two monophyletic groups (the suborder Strepsirrhini, or lemurs, lorises and allies, as well as the tarsiers of the suborder Haplorhini); it is a paraphyletic grouping because it excludes the Simiiformes, which also are descendants of the common ancestor Primates.

  • "monkeys" comprise two monophyletic groups, New World monkeys and Old World monkeys, but is paraphyletic because it excludes hominoids, superfamily Hominoidea, also descendants of the common ancestor Simiiformes.

  • "apes" as a whole, and the "great apes" in particular, are paraphyletic because they exclude humans.

Thus, the members of the two sets of groups, and hence names, do not match, which causes problems in relating scientific names to common (usually traditional) names. Consider the superfamily Hominoidea: In terms of the common names on the right, this group consists of apes and humans and there is no single common name for all the members of the group. One remedy is to create a new common name, in this case "hominoids". Another possibility is to expand the use of one of the traditional names. For example, in his 2005 book, the vertebrate palaeontologist Benton wrote, "The apes, Hominoidea, today include the gibbons and orang-utan ... the gorilla and chimpanzee ... and humans";[10] thereby Benton was using "apes" to mean "hominoids". In that case, the group heretofore called "apes" must now be identified as the "non-human apes".


As of 2005[update], there is no consensus as to which methodology will rule, whether to accept traditional (that is, common), but paraphyletic, names or to use monophyletic names only; or to use 'new' common names or adaptations of old ones. Both competing approaches will be found in biological sources, often in the same work, and sometimes by the same author. Thus, Benton defines "apes" to include humans, then he repeatedly uses "ape-like" to mean "like an ape rather than a human"; and when discussing the reaction of others to a new fossil he writes of "claims that Orrorin ... was an ape rather than a human".[11]



Classification of living primates




A 1927 drawing of chimpanzees, a gibbon (top right) and two orangutans (center and bottom center): The chimpanzee in the upper left is brachiating; the orangutan at the bottom center is knuckle-walking.





Homo sapiens are the only living primate species that is fully bipedal





Nilgiri langur (Trachypithecus johnii), an Old World monkey


A list of the families of the living primates is given below, together with one possible classification into ranks between order and family.[1][8][12][13] Other classifications are also used. For example, an alternative classification of the living Strepsirrhini divides them into two infraorders, Lemuriformes and Lorisiformes.[14]



  • Order Primates

    • Suborder Strepsirrhini: lemurs, galagos and lorisids
      • Infraorder Lemuriformes[a]
        • Superfamily Lemuroidea
          • Family Cheirogaleidae: dwarf lemurs and mouse-lemurs (34 species)

          • Family Daubentoniidae: aye-aye (one species)

          • Family Lemuridae: ring-tailed lemur and allies (21 species)

          • Family Lepilemuridae: sportive lemurs (26 species)

          • Family Indriidae: woolly lemurs and allies (19 species)


        • Superfamily Lorisoidea
          • Family Lorisidae: lorisids (14 species)

          • Family Galagidae: galagos (19 species)




    • Suborder Haplorhini: tarsiers, monkeys and apes
      • Infraorder Tarsiiformes
        • Family Tarsiidae: tarsiers (11 species)

      • Infraorder Simiiformes (or Anthropoidea)
        • Parvorder Platyrrhini: New World monkeys
          • Family Callitrichidae: marmosets and tamarins (42 species)

          • Family Cebidae: capuchins and squirrel monkeys (14 species)

          • Family Aotidae: night or owl monkeys (douroucoulis) (11 species)

          • Family Pitheciidae: titis, sakis and uakaris (43 species)

          • Family Atelidae: howler, spider, woolly spider and woolly monkeys (29 species)


        • Parvorder Catarrhini
          • Superfamily Cercopithecoidea
            • Family Cercopithecidae: Old World monkeys (138 species)

          • Superfamily Hominoidea
            • Family Hylobatidae: gibbons or "lesser apes" (18 species)

            • Family Hominidae: great apes, including humans (8 species)





Order Primates was established by Carl Linnaeus in 1758, in the tenth edition of his book Systema Naturae,[17] for the genera Homo (humans), Simia (other apes and monkeys), Lemur (prosimians) and Vespertilio (bats). In the first edition of the same book (1735), he had used the name Anthropomorpha for Homo, Simia and Bradypus (sloths).[18] In 1839, Henri Marie Ducrotay de Blainville, following Linnaeus and imitating his nomenclature, established the orders Secundates (including the suborders Chiroptera, Insectivora and Carnivora), Tertiates (or Glires) and Quaternates (including Gravigrada, Pachydermata and Ruminantia),[19] but these new taxa were not accepted.


Before Anderson and Jones introduced the classification of Strepsirrhini and Haplorhini in 1984,[20] (followed by McKenna and Bell's 1997 work Classification of Mammals: Above the species level),[21] the Primates were divided into two superfamilies: Prosimii and Anthropoidea.[22] Prosimii included all of the prosimians: Strepsirrhini plus the tarsiers. Anthropoidea contained all of the simians.



Phylogeny and genetics







Euarchontoglires  







Glires 








Rodentia (rodents)





Lagomorpha (rabbits, hares, pikas)




 Euarchonta 








Scandentia (treeshrews)




Primatomorpha








Dermoptera (colugos)




Primates








†Plesiadapiformes





crown primates







Order Primates is part of the clade Euarchontoglires, which is nested within the clade Eutheria of Class Mammalia. Recent molecular genetic research on primates, colugos, and treeshrews has shown that the two species of colugos are more closely related to primates than to treeshrews,[23] even though treeshrews were at one time considered primates.[24] These three orders make up the clade Euarchonta. The combination of this clade with the clade Glires (composed of Rodentia and Lagomorpha) forms the clade Euarchontoglires. Variously, both Euarchonta and Euarchontoglires are ranked as superorders. Some scientists consider Dermoptera to be a suborder of Primates and use the suborder Euprimates for the "true" primates.[25]



Evolution



Hominin timeline


view • discuss • edit


-10 —


-9 —


-8 —


-7 —


-6 —


-5 —


-4 —


-3 —


-2 —


-1 —


0 —




Hominini

Nakalipithecus

Ouranopithecus

Sahelanthropus

Orrorin

Ardipithecus

Australopithecus

Homo habilis

Homo erectus


H. heidelbergensis

Homo sapiens



Neanderthals







Earlier apes




Gorilla split




Possibly bipedal






Chimpanzee split





Earliest bipedal





Stone tools





Exit from Africa




Earliest fire use




Earliest cooking




Earliest clothes




Modern speech





Modern humans



P
l
e
i
s
t
o
c
e
n
e


P
l
i
o
c
e
n
e


M
i
o
c
e
n
e


H

o

m

i

n

i

d

s































(view • discuss)

Axis scale: million years


The image above contains clickable links
Also see: Life timeline and Nature timeline




The primate lineage is thought to go back at least near the Cretaceous–Paleogene boundary or around 63–74 (mya),[26][27][28][29][30] even though the oldest known primates from the fossil record date to the Late Paleocene of Africa (Altiatlasius)[31] or the Paleocene-Eocene transition in the northern continents, c. 55 mya (Cantius, Donrussellia, Altanius, Plesiadapis and Teilhardina).[32][33][34] Other studies, including molecular clock studies, have estimated the origin of the primate branch to have been in the mid-Cretaceous period, around 85 mya.[35][36][37]


By modern cladistic reckoning, the order Primates is monophyletic. The suborder Strepsirrhini, the "wet-nosed" primates, is generally thought to have split off from the primitive primate line about 63 mya,[38] although earlier dates are also supported.[39] The seven strepsirrhine families are the five related lemur families and the two remaining families that include the lorisids and the galagos.[1][12] Older classification schemes wrap Lepilemuridae into Lemuridae and Galagidae into Lorisidae, yielding a four-one family distribution instead of five-two as presented here.[1] During the Eocene, most of the northern continents were dominated by two groups, the adapiforms and the omomyids.[40][41] The former are considered members of Strepsirrhini, but did not have a toothcomb like modern lemurs; recent analysis has demonstrated that Darwinius masillae fits into this grouping.[42] The latter was closely related to tarsiers, monkeys, and apes. How these two groups relate to extant primates is unclear. Omomyids perished about 30 mya,[41] while adapiforms survived until about 10 mya.[43]


According to genetic studies, the lemurs of Madagascar diverged from the lorisoids approximately 75 mya.[39] These studies, as well as chromosomal and molecular evidence, also show that lemurs are more closely related to each other than to other strepsirrhine primates.[39][44] However, Madagascar split from Africa 160 mya and from India 90 mya.[45] To account for these facts, a founding lemur population of a few individuals is thought to have reached Madagascar from Africa via a single rafting event between 50 and 80 mya.[39][44][45] Other colonization options have been examined, such as multiple colonizations from Africa and India, but none are supported by the genetic and molecular evidence.[40]





Common brown lemur, a Strepsirrhine primate


Until recently, the aye-aye has been difficult to place within Strepsirrhini.[1] Theories had been proposed that its family, Daubentoniidae, was either a lemuriform primate (meaning its ancestors split from the lemur line more recently than lemurs and lorises split) or a sister group to all the other strepsirrhines. In 2008, the aye-aye family was confirmed to be most closely related to the other Malagasy lemurs, likely having descended from the same ancestral population that colonized the island.[39]


Suborder Haplorhini, the simple-nosed or "dry-nosed" primates, is composed of two sister clades.[1]Prosimian tarsiers in the family Tarsiidae (monotypic in its own infraorder Tarsiiformes), represent the most basal division, originating about 58 mya.[46][47] The earliest known haplorhine skeleton, that of 55 MA old tarsier-like Archicebus, was found in central China,[48] supporting an already suspected Asian origin for the group.[49] The infraorder Simiiformes (simian primates, consisting of monkeys and apes) emerged about 40 mya,[41] possibly also in Asia; if so, they dispersed across the Tethys Sea from Asia to Africa soon afterwards.[50] There are two simian clades, both parvorders: Catarrhini, which developed in Africa, consisting of Old World monkeys, humans and the other apes, and Platyrrhini, which developed in South America, consisting of New World monkeys.[1] A third clade, which included the eosimiids, developed in Asia, but became extinct millions of years ago.[51]


As in the case of lemurs, the origin of New World monkeys is unclear. Molecular studies of concatenated nuclear sequences have yielded a widely varying estimated date of divergence between platyrrhines and catarrhines, ranging from 33 to 70 mya, while studies based on mitochondrial sequences produce a narrower range of 35 to 43 mya.[33][52] The anthropoid primates possibly traversed the Atlantic Ocean from Africa to South America during the Eocene by island hopping, facilitated by Atlantic Ocean ridges and a lowered sea level.[40] Alternatively, a single rafting event may explain this transoceanic colonization. Due to continental drift, the Atlantic Ocean was not nearly as wide at the time as it is today.[40] Research suggests that a small 1 kg (2.2 lb) primate could have survived 13 days on a raft of vegetation.[53] Given estimated current and wind speeds, this would have provided enough time to make the voyage between the continents.





Emperor tamarin, a New World monkey


Apes and monkeys spread from Africa into Europe and Asia starting in the Miocene.[54] Soon after, the lorises and tarsiers made the same journey. The first hominin fossils were discovered in northern Africa and date back 5–8 mya.[41] Old World monkeys disappeared from Europe about 1.8 mya.[55] Molecular and fossil studies generally show that modern humans originated in Africa 100,000–200,000 years ago.[56]


Although primates are well studied in comparison to other animal groups, several new species have been discovered recently, and genetic tests have revealed previously unrecognised species in known populations. Primate Taxonomy listed about 350 species of primates in 2001;[9] the author, Colin Groves, increased that number to 376 for his contribution to the third edition of Mammal Species of the World (MSW3).[1] However, publications since the taxonomy in MSW3 was compiled in 2003 have pushed the number to 424 species, or 658 including subspecies.[13]



Hybrids


Primate hybrids usually arise in captivity,[57] but there have also been examples in the wild.[58][59] Hybridization occurs where two species' range overlap to form hybrid zones; hybrids may be created by humans when animals are placed in zoos or due to environmental pressures such as predation.[58] Intergeneric hybridizations, hybrids of different genera, have also been found in the wild. Although they belong to genera that have been distinct for several million years, interbreeding still occurs between the gelada and the hamadryas baboon.[60]



Clones


On 24 January 2018, scientists in China reported in the journal Cell the creation of two crab-eating macaque clones, named Zhong Zhong and Hua Hua, using the complex DNA transfer method that produced Dolly the sheep, for the first time.[61][62][63][64][65]



Anatomy and physiology



Eyes


Primates have forward-facing eyes on the front of the skull; binocular vision allows accurate distance perception, useful for the brachiating ancestors of all great apes.[66] A bony ridge above the eye sockets reinforces weaker bones in the face, which are put under strain during chewing. Strepsirrhines have a postorbital bar, a bone around the eye socket, to protect their eyes; in contrast, the higher primates, haplorhines, have evolved fully enclosed sockets.[67]




Primate crania with brain masses indicated



Skull


The primate skull has a large, domed cranium, which is particularly prominent in anthropoids. The cranium protects the large brain, a distinguishing characteristic of this group.[66] The endocranial volume (the volume within the skull) is three times greater in humans than in the greatest nonhuman primate, reflecting a larger brain size.[68] The mean endocranial volume is 1,201 cubic centimeters in humans, 469 cm3 in gorillas, 400 cm3 in chimpanzees and 397 cm3 in orangutans.[68] The primary evolutionary trend of primates has been the elaboration of the brain, in particular the neocortex (a part of the cerebral cortex), which is involved with sensory perception, generation of motor commands, spatial reasoning, conscious thought and, in humans, language.[69] While other mammals rely heavily on their sense of smell, the arboreal life of primates has led to a tactile, visually dominant sensory system,[69] a reduction in the olfactory region of the brain and increasingly complex social behavior.[70]




An 1893 drawing of the hands and feet of various primates



Hands


Primates generally have five digits on each limb (pentadactyly), with keratin nails on the end of each finger and toe. The bottom sides of the hands and feet have sensitive pads on the fingertips. Most have opposable thumbs, a characteristic primate feature most developed in humans, though not limited to this order, (opossums and koalas, for example, also have them).[66] Thumbs allow some species to use tools. In primates, the combination of opposing thumbs, short fingernails (rather than claws) and long, inward-closing fingers is a relict of the ancestral practice of gripping branches, and has, in part, allowed some species to develop brachiation (swinging by the arms from tree limb to tree limb) as a significant means of locomotion. Prosimians have clawlike nails on the second toe of each foot, called toilet-claws, which they use for grooming.[66]




Vervet hindfoot showing fingerprint ridges on the sole



Torso


The primate collar bone is a prominent element of the pectoral girdle; this allows the shoulder joint broad mobility.[71] Compared to Old World monkeys, apes have more mobile shoulder joints and arms due to the dorsal position of the scapula, broad ribcages that are flatter front-to-back, a shorter, less mobile spine, and with lower vertebrae greatly reduced - resulting in tail loss in some species. Prehensile tails are found in atelids, including the howler, spider, woolly spider, woolly monkeys; and in capuchins. [72][73] Male primates have a pendulous penis and scrotal testes.[74][75]



Face


Primates show an evolutionary trend towards a reduced snout.[71] Technically, Old World monkeys are distinguished from New World monkeys by the structure of the nose, and from apes by the arrangement of their teeth.[70] In New World monkeys, the nostrils face sideways; in Old World monkeys, they face downwards.[70] Dental pattern in primates vary considerably; although some have lost most of their incisors, all retain at least one lower incisor.[70] In most strepsirrhines, the lower incisors and canines form a toothcomb, which is used in grooming and sometimes foraging,[70][75] and the first lower premolar is shaped like a canine.[75] Old World monkeys have eight premolars, compared with 12 in New World monkeys.[70] The Old World species are divided into apes and monkeys depending on the number of cusps on their molars: monkeys have four, apes have five[70] - although humans may have four or five.[76] The main hominid molar cusp (hypocone) evolved in early primate history, while the cusp of the corresponding primitive lower molar (paraconid) was lost. Prosimians are distinguished by their immobilized upper lips, the moist tip of their noses and forward-facing lower front teeth.




The tapetum lucidum of a northern greater galago, typical of prosimians, reflects the light of the photographers flash



Color vision


The evolution of color vision in primates is unique among most eutherian mammals. While the remote vertebrate ancestors of the primates possessed three color vision (trichromaticism), the nocturnal, warm-blooded, mammalian ancestors lost one of three cones in the retina during the Mesozoic era. Fish, reptiles and birds are therefore trichromatic or tetrachromatic, while all mammals, with the exception of some primates and marsupials,[77] are dichromats or monochromats (totally color blind).[75] Nocturnal primates, such as the night monkeys and bush babies, are often monochromatic. Catarrhines are routinely trichromatic due to a gene duplication of the red-green opsin gene at the base of their lineage, 30 to 40 million years ago.[75][78] Platyrrhines, on the other hand, are trichromatic in a few cases only.[79] Specifically, individual females must be heterozygous for two alleles of the opsin gene (red and green) located on the same locus of the X chromosome.[75] Males, therefore, can only be dichromatic, while females can be either dichromatic or trichromatic. Color vision in strepsirrhines is not as well understood; however, research indicates a range of color vision similar to that found in platyrrhines.[75]


Like catarrhines, howler monkeys (a family of platyrrhines) show routine trichromatism that has been traced to an evolutionarily recent gene duplication.[80] Howler monkeys are one of the most specialized leaf-eaters of the New World monkeys; fruits are not a major part of their diets,[81] and the type of leaves they prefer to consume (young, nutritive, and digestible) are detectable only by a red-green signal. Field work exploring the dietary preferences of howler monkeys suggests that routine trichromaticism was selected by environment.[79]



Sexual dimorphism





Distinct sexual size dimorphism can be seen between the female and two male hamadryas baboons.


Sexual dimorphism is often exhibited in simians, though to a greater degree in Old World species (apes and some monkeys) than New World species. Recent studies involve comparing DNA to examine both the variation in the expression of the dimorphism among primates and the fundamental causes of sexual dimorphism. Primates usually have dimorphism in body mass[82][83][84] and canine tooth size[85][86][87] along with pelage and skin color.[88] The dimorphism can be attributed to and affected by different factors, including mating system,[82][89] size,[89] habitat and diet.[90]


Comparative analyses have generated a more complete understanding of the relationship between sexual selection, natural selection, and mating systems in primates. Studies have shown that dimorphism is the product of changes in both male and female traits.[82][91] Ontogenetic scaling, where relative extension of a common growth trajectory occurs, may give some insight into the relationship between sexual dimorphism and growth patterns.[92] Some evidence from the fossil record suggests that there was convergent evolution of dimorphism, and some extinct hominids probably had greater dimorphism than any living primate.[91]



Locomotion





Diademed sifaka, a lemur that is a vertical clinger and leaper


Primate species move by brachiation, bipedalism, leaping, arboreal and terrestrial quadrupedalism, climbing, knuckle-walking or by a combination of these methods. Several prosimians are primarily vertical clingers and leapers. These include many bushbabies, all indriids (i.e., sifakas, avahis and indris), sportive lemurs, and all tarsiers.[93] Other prosimians are arboreal quadrupeds and climbers. Some are also terrestrial quadrupeds, while some are leapers. Most monkeys are both arboreal and terrestrial quadrupeds and climbers. Gibbons, muriquis and spider monkeys all brachiate extensively,[55] with gibbons sometimes doing so in remarkably acrobatic fashion. Woolly monkeys also brachiate at times.[81]Orangutans use a similar form of locomotion called quadramanous climbing, in which they use their arms and legs to carry their heavy bodies through the trees.[55] Chimpanzees and gorillas knuckle walk,[55] and can move bipedally for short distances. Although numerous species, such as australopithecines and early hominids, have exhibited fully bipedal locomotion, humans are the only extant species with this trait.[94]



Behavior



Social systems


Primates are among the most social of animals, forming pairs or family groups, uni-male harems, and multi-male/multi-female groups.[95]Richard Wrangham stated that social systems of non-human primates are best classified by the amount of movement by females occurring between groups.[96] He proposed four categories:


  • Female transfer systems – females move away from the group in which they were born. Females of a group will not be closely related whereas males will have remained with their natal groups, and this close association may be influential in social behavior. The groups formed are generally quite small. This organization can be seen in chimpanzees, where the males, who are typically related, will cooperate in defense of the group's territory. It is claimed that the practice was also prevalent in some Neanderthal populations. A 49,000-year-old grave was found in Spain in 2010 which contained three related-to-each-other males, with three unrelated-to-each-other females, suggesting they were the partners of the males.[97] A 2011 study using ratios of strontium isotopes in teeth also suggested that roughly 2 million years ago, among Australopithecus and Paranthropus robustus groups in southern Africa, females tended to settle farther from their region of birth than males did.[98][99] Among New World Monkeys, spider monkeys and muriquis use this system.[100]


A social huddle of ring-tailed lemurs. The two individuals on the right exposing their white ventral surface are sunning themselves.


  • Male transfer systems – while the females remain in their natal groups, the males will emigrate as adolescents. Polygynous and multi-male societies are classed in this category. Group sizes are usually larger. This system is common among the ring-tailed lemur, capuchin monkeys and cercopithecine monkeys.[55]

  • Monogamous species – a male–female bond, sometimes accompanied by a juvenile offspring. There is shared responsibility of parental care and territorial defense. The offspring leaves the parents' territory during adolescence. Gibbons essentially use this system, although "monogamy" in this context does not necessarily mean absolute sexual fidelity.[101] These species do not live in larger groups.

  • Solitary species – often males who defend territories that include the home ranges of several females. This type of organization is found in the prosimians such as the slow loris. Orangutans do not defend their territory but effectively have this organization.[102]

Other systems are known to occur as well. For example, with howler monkeys both the males and females typically transfer from their natal group on reaching sexual maturity, resulting in groups in which neither the males nor females are typically related.[81] Some prosimians, colobine monkeys and callitrichid monkeys use this system.[55]


The transfer of females or males from their native group is likely an adaptation for avoiding inbreeding.[103] An analysis of breeding records of captive primate colonies representing numerous different species indicates that the infant mortality of inbred young is generally higher than that of non-inbred young.[103][104] This effect of inbreeding on infant mortality is probably largely a result of increased expression of deleterious recessive alleles (see Inbreeding depression).




The chimpanzees are social great apes.


Primatologist Jane Goodall, who studied in the Gombe Stream National Park, noted fission-fusion societies in chimpanzees.[105] There is fission when the main group splits up to forage during the day, then fusion when the group returns at night to sleep as a group. This social structure can also be observed in the hamadryas baboon,[106]spider monkeys[81] and the bonobo.[106] The gelada has a similar social structure in which many smaller groups come together to form temporary herds of up to 600 monkeys.[106]


These social systems are affected by three main ecological factors: distribution of resources, group size, and predation.[107] Within a social group there is a balance between cooperation and competition. Cooperative behaviors include social grooming (removing skin parasites and cleaning wounds), food sharing, and collective defense against predators or of a territory. Aggressive behaviors often signal competition for food, sleeping sites or mates. Aggression is also used in establishing dominance hierarchies.[107][108]



Interspecific associations


Several species of primates are known to associate in the wild. Some of these associations have been extensively studied. In the Tai Forest of Africa several species coordinate anti-predator behavior. These include the Diana monkey, Campbell's mona monkey, lesser spot-nosed monkey, western red colobus, king colobus and sooty mangabey, which coordinate anti-predator alarm calls.[109] Among the predators of these monkeys is the common chimpanzee.[110]


The red-tailed monkey associates with several species, including the western red colobus, blue monkey, Wolf's mona monkey, mantled guereza, black crested mangabey and Allen's swamp monkey.[106] Several of these species are preyed upon by the common chimpanzee.[111]


In South America, squirrel monkeys associate with capuchin monkeys.[112] This may have more to do with foraging benefits to the squirrel monkeys than anti-predation benefits.[112]



Cognition and communication






A pair of black howler monkeys vocalizing.


Primates have advanced cognitive abilities: some make tools and use them to acquire food and for social displays;[113][114] some have sophisticated hunting strategies requiring cooperation, influence and rank;[115] they are status conscious, manipulative and capable of deception;[116] they can recognise kin and conspecifics;[117][118] and they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax and concepts of number and numerical sequence.[119][120][121] Research in primate cognition explores problem solving, memory, social interaction, a theory of mind, and numerical, spatial, and abstract concepts.[122] Comparative studies show a trend towards higher intelligence going from prosimians to New World monkeys to Old World monkeys, and significantly higher average cognitive abilities in the great apes.[123][124] However, there is a great deal of variation in each group (e.g., among New World monkeys, both spider[123] and capuchin monkeys[124] have scored highly by some measures), as well as in the results of different studies.[123][124]


Lemurs, lorises, tarsiers, and New World monkeys rely on olfactory signals for many aspects of social and reproductive behavior.[69] Specialized glands are used to mark territories with pheromones, which are detected by the vomeronasal organ; this process forms a large part of the communication behavior of these primates.[69] In Old World monkeys and apes this ability is mostly vestigial, having regressed as trichromatic eyes evolved to become the main sensory organ.[125] Primates also use vocalizations, gestures, and facial expressions to convey psychological state.[126] Facial musculature is complex in primates, particularly in monkeys and apes. Like humans, chimpanzees can distinguish the faces of familiar and unfamiliar individuals.[127] Hand and arm gestures are also important forms of communication for great apes and a single gesture can have multiple functions.[128]




The Philippine tarsier, has a high-frequency limit of auditory sensitivity of approximately 91 kHz with a dominant frequency of 70 kHz. Such values are among the highest recorded for any terrestrial mammal, and a relatively extreme example of ultrasonic communication. For Philippine tarsiers, ultrasonic vocalizations might represent a private channel of communication that subverts detection by predators, prey and competitors, enhances energetic efficiency, or improves detection against low-frequency background noise.[129] Male howler monkeys are among the loudest land mammals and their roars can be heard up to 4.8 km (3.0 mi).[130] Roars are produced by modified larynx and enlarged hyoid bone which contains an air sac.[131] These calls are thought to relate to intergroup spacing and territorial protection as well as possibly mate-guarding.[132] The vervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of at least four different predators, and the reactions of other monkeys vary according to the call. For example, if an alarm call signals a python, the monkeys climb into the trees, whereas the eagle alarm causes monkeys to seek a hiding place on the ground.[133] Many non-human primates have the vocal anatomy to produce human speech but lack the proper brain wiring.[134] Vowel-like vocal patterns have been recorded in baboons which has implications for the origin of speech in humans.[135]



Aggression


Non-human primates and humans have been observed to be very similar in terms of personality, such as chimpanzees having “’Big Five’ personality factors found in humans, i.e. neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness”. Primates seem to possess a sixth personality trait, dominance.[136] Chimpanzees and bonobos tend to be more social than male orangutans, who cannot be in the same living space. Male gorillas display aggression during a majority of their interactions while chimpanzees reconcile after their aggressive encounters. Killing males in differing groups in order to expand territory is an act of violence only seen in chimps and humans. Bonobos tend to live in cohesive communities with lower aggressive incidents.[137] This reduction in reactive aggression committed by male bonobos may be due to the evolution of prominent female groups.


Both humans and nonhuman primates, such as chimpanzees, exhibit proactive aggression, a type of preplanned aggression with a reward. This aggression is expressed between neighboring groups. Proactive aggression ultimately increases the fitness of the community as a whole as the size of the community increases. Unlike proactive aggression, reactive aggression is low in humans but high in chimpanzees. Reactive aggression is a result of anger in order to cease a stressful stimulus. Low reactive aggression in humans can be attributed to tolerance and cooperation. Wrangham found “evolution of within-group tolerance, such as individual selection for cooperative breeding, group selection for parochial altruism, and cultural group selection for prosocial norms”. Ranking in nonhuman primates stems from the most aggressive male, while nomadic hunter-gatherers are respected for their prestige and ability to form alliances and negotiations. Boehm proposed the “execution hypothesis”, or the approval by the community of killing group members who violate the norm of the group or are deemed a bully. Hunter-gatherers who attempted to utilize aggression were viewed as a threat and removed from the group. The removal of community threats can be likened to modern capital punishment, more specifically the death penalty.[138]



Life history




A crab-eating macaque breastfeeding her baby


Primates have slower rates of development than other mammals.[55] All primate infants are breastfed by their mothers (with the exception of some human cultures and various zoo raised primates which are fed formula) and rely on them for grooming and transportation.[55] In some species, infants are protected and transported by males in the group, particularly males who may be their fathers.[55] Other relatives of the infant, such as siblings and aunts, may participate in its care as well.[55] Most primate mothers cease ovulation while breastfeeding an infant; once the infant is weaned the mother can reproduce again.[55] This often leads to weaning conflict with infants who attempt to continue breastfeeding.[55]


Infanticide is common in polygynous species such as gray langurs and gorillas. Adult males may kill dependent offspring that are not theirs so the female will return to estrus and thus they can sire offspring of their own. Social monogamy in some species may have evolved to combat this behavior.[139]Promiscuity may also lessen the risk of infanticide since paternity becomes uncertain.[140]


Primates have a longer juvenile period between weaning and sexual maturity than other mammals of similar size.[55] Some primates such as galagos and new world monkeys use tree-holes for nesting, and park juveniles in leafy patches while foraging. Other primates follow a strategy of "riding", i.e. carrying individuals on the body while feeding. Adults may construct or use nesting sites, sometimes accompanied by juveniles, for the purpose of resting, a behavior which has developed secondarily in the great apes.[141][142] During the juvenile period, primates are more susceptible than adults to predation and starvation; they gain experience in feeding and avoiding predators during this time.[55] They learn social and fighting skills, often through playing.[55] Primates, especially females, have longer lifespans than other similarly sized mammals,[55] this may be partially due to their slower metabolisms.[143] Late in life, female catarrhine primates appear to undergo a cessation of reproductive function known as menopause; other groups are less studied.[144]



Diet, feeding and hunting




Leaf eating mantled guereza, a species of black-and-white colobus




A mouse lemur holds a cut piece of fruit in its hands and eats


Primates exploit a variety of food sources. It has been said that many characteristics of modern primates, including humans, derive from an early ancestor's practice of taking most of its food from the tropical canopy.[145] Most primates include fruit in their diets to obtain easily digested nutrients including carbohydrates and lipids for energy.[55] Primates in the suborder Strepsirrhini (non-tarsier prosimians) are able to synthesize vitamin C, like most other mammals, while primates of the suborder Haplorrhini (tarsiers, monkeys and apes) have lost this ability, and require the vitamin in their diet.[146]


Many primates have anatomical specializations that enable them to exploit particular foods, such as fruit, leaves, gum or insects.[55] For example, leaf eaters such as howler monkeys, black-and-white colobuses and sportive lemurs have extended digestive tracts which enable them to absorb nutrients from leaves that can be difficult to digest.[55]Marmosets, which are gum eaters, have strong incisor teeth, enabling them to open tree bark to get to the gum, and claws rather than nails, enabling them to cling to trees while feeding.[55] The aye-aye combines rodent-like teeth with a long, thin middle finger to fill the same ecological niche as a woodpecker. It taps on trees to find insect larvae, then gnaws holes in the wood and inserts its elongated middle finger to pull the larvae out.[147] Some species have additional specializations. For example, the grey-cheeked mangabey has thick enamel on its teeth, enabling it to open hard fruits and seeds that other monkeys cannot.[55] The gelada is the only primate species that feeds primarily on grass.[148]



Hunting



Portrait of a Dayak hunter in Borneo with a boar over his shoulder

Humans have traditionally hunted prey for subsistence.


Tarsiers are the only extant obligate carnivorous primates, exclusively eating insects, crustaceans, small vertebrates and snakes (including venomous species).[149]Capuchin monkeys can exploit many different types of plant matter, including fruit, leaves, flowers, buds, nectar and seeds, but also eat insects and other invertebrates, bird eggs, and small vertebrates such as birds, lizards, squirrels and bats.[81]


The common chimpanzee eats an omnivorous frugivorous diet. It prefers fruit above all other food items and even seeks out and eats them when they are not abundant. It also eats leaves and leaf buds, seeds, blossoms, stems, pith, bark and resin. Insects and meat make up a small proportion of their diet, estimated as 2%.[150][151] The meat consumption includes predation on other primate species, such as the western red colobus monkey.[110] This sometimes involves tool use. Common chimpanzees sharpen sticks to use as weapons when hunting mammals. This is considered the first evidence of systematic use of weapons in a species other than humans. Researchers documented 22 occasions where wild chimpanzees fashioned sticks into "spears" to hunt lesser bush babies (Galago senegalensis). In each case, a chimpanzee modified a branch by breaking off one or both ends and, frequently using its teeth, sharpened the stick. The tools, on average, were about 60 cm (24 in) long and 1.1 cm (0.4 in) in circumference. The chimpanzees then jabbed their spears into hollows in tree trunks where bush babies slept. There was a single case in which a chimpanzee successfully extracted a bush baby with the tool. The bonobo is an omnivorous frugivore – the majority of its diet is fruit, but it supplements this with leaves, meat from small vertebrates, such as anomalures, flying squirrels and duikers,[152] and invertebrates.[153] In some instances, bonobos have been shown to consume lower-order primates.[154][155]



As prey


Predators of primates include various species of carnivorans, birds of prey, reptiles and other primates. Even gorillas have been recorded as prey. Predators of primates have diverse hunting strategies and as such, primates have evolved several different antipredator adaptations including crypsis, alarm calls and mobbing. Several species have separate alarm calls for different predators such as air-borne or ground-dwelling predators. Predation may have shaped group size in primates as species exposed to higher predation pressures appear to live in larger groups.[156] With their technology and increased intelligence, modern humans are virtually free of threats from other predators and are themselves apex predators.



Tool use and manufacture




Tool use




A Western lowland gorilla using a stick possibly to gauge the depth of water





Crab-eating macaques with stone tools


There are many reports of non-human primates using tools, both in the wild or when captive. The use of tools by primates is varied and includes hunting (mammals, invertebrates, fish), collecting honey, processing food (nuts, fruits, vegetables and seeds), collecting water, weapons and shelter.


In 1960, Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee poking pieces of grass into a termite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to “fish” or "dip" for termites.[157] There are more limited reports of the closely related bonobo using tools in the wild; it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity.[158] It has been reported that females, both chimpanzee and bonobo, use tools more avidly than males.[159] Orangutans in Borneo scoop catfish out of small ponds. Anthropologist Anne Russon saw several animals on these forested islands learn on their own to jab at catfish with sticks, so that the panicked prey would flop out of ponds and into the orangutan's waiting hands[160] There are few reports of gorillas using tools in the wild. An adult female Western lowland gorilla used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her in crossing a pool of water. Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering, and another used a log as a bridge.[161]


The black-striped capuchin was the first non-ape primate for which tool use was documented in the wild; individuals were observed cracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone.[162] In Thailand and Myanmar, crab-eating macaques use stone tools to open nuts, oysters and other bivalves, and various types of sea snails.[163] Chacma baboons use stones as weapons; stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat to when they are threatened. Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side whereupon they tumble down the side of the cliff or fall directly to the canyon floor.[164]


Although they have not been observed to use tools in the wild, lemurs in controlled settings have been shown to be capable of understanding the functional properties of the objects they had been trained to use as tools, performing as well as tool-using haplorhines.[165]



Tool manufacture


Tool manufacture is much rarer than simple tool use and probably represents higher cognitive functioning. Soon after her initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals.[157] Both bonobos and chimpanzees have also been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water and are used as grooming tools. Sumatran orangutans have been observed making and using tools. They will break off a tree branch that is about 30 cm long, snap off the twigs, fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites.[166][167] In the wild, mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at Chester Zoo (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails.[168] Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools.[169]



Ecology






Rhesus macaque at Agra Fort, India


Primates evolved from arboreal animals, and many species live most of their lives in trees. Most primate species live in tropical rain forests. The number of primate species within tropical areas has been shown to be positively correlated to the amount of rainfall and the amount of rain forest area.[170] Accounting for 25% to 40% of the fruit-eating animals (by weight) within tropical rainforests, primates play an important ecological role by dispersing seeds of many tree species.[171]


Some species are partially terrestrial, such as baboons and patas monkeys, and a few species are fully terrestrial, such as geladas and humans. Non-human primates live in a diverse number of forested habitats in the tropical latitudes of Africa, India, Southeast Asia, and South America, including rainforests, mangrove forests, and montane forests. There are some examples of non-human primates that live outside of the tropics; the mountain-dwelling Japanese macaque lives in the north of Honshū where there is snow-cover eight months of the year; the Barbary macaque lives in the Atlas Mountains of Algeria and Morocco. Primate habitats span a range of altitudes: the black snub-nosed monkey has been found living in the Hengduan Mountains at altitudes of 4,700 meters (15,400 ft),[172] the mountain gorilla can be found at 4,200 meters (13,200 ft) crossing the Virunga Mountains,[173] and the gelada has been found at elevations of up to 5,000 m (16,000 ft) in the Ethiopian Highlands. Although most species are generally shy of water, a few are good swimmers and are comfortable in swamps and watery areas, including the proboscis monkey, De Brazza's monkey and Allen's swamp monkey, which has developed small webbing between its fingers. Some primates, such as the rhesus macaque and gray langurs, can exploit human-modified environments and even live in cities.[106][174]




Interactions between humans and other primates



Disease transmission





Slow lorises are popular in the exotic pet trade, which threatens wild populations.


Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create pathways for the transmission of zoonotic diseases. Viruses such as Herpesviridae (most notably Herpes B Virus), Poxviridae, measles, ebola, rabies, the Marburg virus and viral hepatitis can be transmitted to humans; in some cases the viruses produce potentially fatal diseases in both humans and non-human primates.[175]



Legal and social status



Only humans are recognized as persons and protected in law by the United Nations Universal Declaration of Human Rights.[b] The legal status of NHPs, on the other hand, is the subject of much debate, with organizations such as the Great Ape Project (GAP) campaigning to award at least some of them legal rights.[177] In June 2008, Spain became the first country in the world to recognize the rights of some NHPs, when its parliament's cross-party environmental committee urged the country to comply with GAP's recommendations, which are that chimpanzees, bonobos, orangutans, and gorillas are not to be used for animal experiments.[178][179]


Many species of NHP are kept as pets by humans, the Allied Effort to Save Other Primates (AESOP) estimates that around 15,000 NHPs live as exotic pets in the United States.[180] The expanding Chinese middle class has increased demand for NHPs as exotic pets in recent years.[181] Although NHP import for the pet trade was banned in the U.S. in 1975, smuggling still occurs along the United States – Mexico border, with prices ranging from US$3000 for monkeys to $30,000 for apes.[182]




Sam, a rhesus macaque, was flown into space by NASA in 1959


Primates are used as model organisms in laboratories and have been used in space missions.[183] They serve as service animals for disabled humans. Capuchin monkeys can be trained to assist quadriplegic humans; their intelligence, memory, and manual dexterity make them ideal helpers.[184]


NHPs are kept in zoos around the globe. Historically, zoos were primarily a form of entertainment, but more recently have shifted their focus towards conservation, education and research. GAP does not insist that all NHPs should be released from zoos, primarily because captive-born primates lack the knowledge and experience to survive in the wild if released.[185]



Role in scientific research



Thousands of non-human primates are used around the world in research because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans.[186][187] In particular, the brains and eyes of NHPs more closely parallel human anatomy than those of any other animals. NHPs are commonly used in preclinical trials, neuroscience, ophthalmology studies, and toxicity studies. Rhesus macaques are often used, as are other macaques, African green monkeys, chimpanzees, baboons, squirrel monkeys, and marmosets, both wild-caught and purpose-bred.[186][188]


In 2005, GAP reported that 1,280 of the 3,100 NHPs living in captivity in the United States were used for experiments.[177] In 2004, the European Union used around 10,000 NHPs in such experiments; in 2005 in Great Britain, 4,652 experiments were conducted on 3,115 NHPs.[189] Governments of many nations have strict care requirements of NHPs kept in captivity. In the US, federal guidelines extensively regulate aspects of NHP housing, feeding, enrichment, and breeding.[190] European groups such as the European Coalition to End Animal Experiments are seeking a ban on all NHP use in experiments as part of the European Union's review of animal testing legislation.[191]



Extinction threats




Humans are known to hunt other primates for food, so-called bushmeat. Pictured are two men who have killed a number of silky sifaka and white-headed brown lemurs.


The International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) lists more than a third of primates as critically endangered or vulnerable. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction, including: 87% of species in Madagascar, 73% in Asia, 37% in Africa, and 36% in South and Central America.[192] Additionally, 75% of primate species have decreasing populations.[192] Trade is regulated, as all species are listed by CITES in Appendix II, except 50 species and subspecies listed in Appendix I, which gain full protection from trade.[193][194]


Common threats to primate species include deforestation, forest fragmentation, monkey drives (resulting from primate crop raiding),[195] and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing is widely regarded as the process that most threatens primates.[196][197][198] More than 90% of primate species occur in tropical forests.[197][199] The main cause of forest loss is clearing for agriculture, although commercial logging, subsistence harvesting of timber, mining, and dam construction also contribute to tropical forest destruction.[199] In Indonesia large areas of lowland forest have been cleared to increase palm oil production, and one analysis of satellite imagery concluded that during 1998 and 1999 there was a loss of 1,000 Sumatran orangutans per year in the Leuser Ecosystem alone.[200]




The critically endangered Sumatran orangutan


Primates with a large body size (over 5 kg) are at increased extinction risk due to their greater profitability to poachers compared to smaller primates.[199] They reach sexual maturity later and have a longer period between births. Populations therefore recover more slowly after being depleted by poaching or the pet trade.[201] Data for some African cities show that half of all protein consumed in urban areas comes from the bushmeat trade.[202] Endangered primates such as guenons and the drill are hunted at levels that far exceed sustainable levels.[202] This is due to their large body size, ease of transport and profitability per animal.[202] As farming encroaches on forest habitats, primates feed on the crops, causing the farmers large economic losses.[203] Primate crop raiding gives locals a negative impression of primates, hindering conservation efforts.[204]


Madagascar, home to five endemic primate families, has experienced the greatest extinction of the recent past; since human settlement 1,500 years ago, at least eight classes and fifteen of the larger species have become extinct due to hunting and habitat destruction.[69] Among the primates wiped out were Archaeoindris (a lemur larger than a silverback gorilla) and the families Palaeopropithecidae and Archaeolemuridae.[69]


In Asia, Hinduism, Buddhism, and Islam prohibit eating primate meat; however, primates are still hunted for food.[199] Some smaller traditional religions allow the consumption of primate meat.[205][206] The pet trade and traditional medicine also increase demand for illegal hunting.[181][207][208] The rhesus macaque, a model organism, was protected after excessive trapping threatened its numbers in the 1960s; the program was so effective that they are now viewed as a pest throughout their range.[198]




The critically endangered silky sifaka


In Central and South America forest fragmentation and hunting are the two main problems for primates. Large tracts of forest are now rare in Central America.[196][209] This increases the amount of forest vulnerable to edge effects such as farmland encroachment, lower levels of humidity and a change in plant life.[210][211] Movement restriction results in a greater amount of inbreeding, which can cause deleterious effects leading to a population bottleneck, whereby a significant percentage of the population is lost.[212][213]


There are 21 critically endangered primates, 7 of which have remained on the IUCN's "The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates" list since the year 2000: the silky sifaka, Delacour's langur, the white-headed langur, the gray-shanked douc, the Tonkin snub-nosed monkey, the Cross River gorilla and the Sumatran orangutan.[214]Miss Waldron's red colobus was recently declared extinct when no trace of the subspecies could be found from 1993 to 1999.[215] A few hunters have found and killed individuals since then, but the subspecies' prospects remain bleak.[216]



See also




  • Arboreal theory

  • Human evolution

  • International Primate Day

  • List of primates

  • List of fossil primates

  • Nest-building in primates

  • Primatology



Footnotes




  1. ^ ab Although the monophyletic relationship between lemurs and lorisoids is widely accepted, their clade name is not. The term "lemuriform" is used here because it derives from one popular taxonomy that clumps the clade of toothcombed primates into one infraorder and the extinct, non-toothcombed adapiforms into another, both within the suborder Strepsirrhini.[15][16] However, another popular alternative taxonomy places the lorisoids in their own infraorder, Lorisiformes.[14]


  2. ^ Article 6: Everyone has the right to recognition everywhere as a person before the law.[176]




References




  1. ^ abcdefgh Groves, C.P. (2005). Wilson, D.E.; Reeder, D.M., eds. Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 111–184. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494..mw-parser-output cite.citationfont-style:inherit.mw-parser-output .citation qquotes:"""""""'""'".mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-free abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/6/65/Lock-green.svg/9px-Lock-green.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-limited a,.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-registration abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/d/d6/Lock-gray-alt-2.svg/9px-Lock-gray-alt-2.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-subscription abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/a/aa/Lock-red-alt-2.svg/9px-Lock-red-alt-2.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registrationcolor:#555.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription span,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registration spanborder-bottom:1px dotted;cursor:help.mw-parser-output .cs1-ws-icon abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/4/4c/Wikisource-logo.svg/12px-Wikisource-logo.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output code.cs1-codecolor:inherit;background:inherit;border:inherit;padding:inherit.mw-parser-output .cs1-hidden-errordisplay:none;font-size:100%.mw-parser-output .cs1-visible-errorfont-size:100%.mw-parser-output .cs1-maintdisplay:none;color:#33aa33;margin-left:0.3em.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registration,.mw-parser-output .cs1-formatfont-size:95%.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-left,.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-wl-leftpadding-left:0.2em.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-right,.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-wl-rightpadding-right:0.2em


  2. ^ "Primate". Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary. Merriam-Webster. Retrieved 2008-07-21.


  3. ^ The Book of Popular Science. 1963. p. 257.


  4. ^ Anon. (1911), "Ape", Encyclopædia Britannica, XIX (11th ed.), New York: Encyclopædia Britannica, retrieved 2011-07-10


  5. ^ Dixson, A.F. (1981), The Natural History of the Gorilla, London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson, ISBN 978-0-297-77895-0


  6. ^ Definitions of paraphyly vary; for the one used here see e.g. Stace, Clive A. (2010), "Classification by molecules: What's in it for field botanists?" (PDF), Watsonia, 28: 103–122, archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-26, retrieved 2010-02-07.


  7. ^ Definitions of monophyly vary; for the one used here see e.g. Mishler, Brent D (2009), "Species are not Uniquely Real Biological Entities", in Ayala, F.J. & Arp, R., Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Biology, pp. 110–122, doi:10.1002/9781444314922.ch6, ISBN 978-1-4443-1492-2.


  8. ^ ab Cartmill, M.; Smith, F. H. (2011). The Human Lineage. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-118-21145-8.


  9. ^ ab Groves, C. P. (2001). Primate Taxonomy. Smithsonian Institution Press. ISBN 1-56098-872-X.


  10. ^ Benton 2005, p. 371.


  11. ^ Benton 2005, pp. 378–380.


  12. ^ ab Mittermeier, R.; Ganzhorn, J.; Konstant, W.; Glander, K.; Tattersall, I.; Groves, C.; Rylands, A.; Hapke, A.; Ratsimbazafy, J.; Mayor, M.; Louis, E.; Rumpler, Y.; Schwitzer, C.; Rasoloarison, R. (December 2008). "Lemur Diversity in Madagascar". International Journal of Primatology. 29 (6): 1607–1656. doi:10.1007/s10764-008-9317-y.


  13. ^ ab Rylands, A. B. & Mittermeier, R. A. (2009). "The Diversity of the New World Primates (Platyrrhini)". In Garber, P. A.; Estrada, A.; Bicca-Marques, J. C.; Heymann, E. W. & Strier, K. B. South American Primates: Comparative Perspectives in the Study of Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. Springer. ISBN 978-0-387-78704-6.


  14. ^ ab Hartwig 2011, pp. 20–21.


  15. ^ Szalay & Delson 1980, p. 149.


  16. ^ Cartmill 2010, p. 15.


  17. ^ Linnaeus, C. (1758). Sistema naturae per regna tria Naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus differentiis, synonimis locis. Tomus I. Impensis direct. Laurentii Salvii, Holmia. pp. 20–32.


  18. ^ Linnaeus, C. (1735). Sistema naturae sive regna tria Naturae systematice proposita per classes, ordines, genera, & species. apud Theodorum Haak, Lugduni Batavorum. pp. s.p.


  19. ^ Blainville, H. (1839). "Nouvelle classification des Mammifères". Annales Françaises et Etrangères d'Anatomie et de Physiologie Appliquées à la Médicine et à l'Histoire Naturelle, 3. pp. 268–269.


  20. ^ Thorington, R. W. & Anderson, S. (1984). "Primates". In Anderson, S. & Jones, J. K. Orders and Families of Recent Mammals of the World. New York: John Wiley and Sons. pp. 187–217. ISBN 978-0-471-08493-8.


  21. ^ McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997). Classification of Mammals: Above the species level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 631. ISBN 0-231-11013-8.


  22. ^ Strier, K. (2007). Primate Behavioral Ecology (Third ed.). pp. 50–53. ISBN 0-205-44432-6.


  23. ^ Janečka, J. E.; Miller, W.; Pringle, T. H.; Wiens, F.; Zitzmann, A.; Helgen, K. M.; Springer, M. S.; Murphy, W. J. (2 November 2007). "Molecular and Genomic Data Identify the Closest Living Relative of Primates". Science. 318 (5851): 792–794. Bibcode:2007Sci...318..792J. doi:10.1126/science.1147555. PMID 17975064. Archived from the original on 2008-06-21. Retrieved 2008-08-17. (Subscription required (help)).


  24. ^ Kavanagh, M. (1983). A Complete Guide to Monkeys, Apes and Other Primates. New York: Viking Press. p. 18. ISBN 0-670-43543-0.


  25. ^ McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997). Classification of Mammals Above the Species Level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 329. ISBN 0-231-11012-X.


  26. ^ Williams, B.A.; Kay, R.F.; Kirk, E.C. (2010). "New perspectives on anthropoid origins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (11): 4797–4804. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107. PMC 2841917. PMID 20212104.


  27. ^ Stanyon, Roscoe; Springer, Mark S.; Meredith, Robert W.; Gatesy, John; Emerling, Christopher A.; Park, Jong; Rabosky, Daniel L.; Stadler, Tanja; Steiner, Cynthia; Ryder, Oliver A.; Janečka, Jan E.; Fisher, Colleen A.; Murphy, William J. (2012). "Macroevolutionary Dynamics and Historical Biogeography of Primate Diversification Inferred from a Species Supermatrix". PLOS One. 7 (11): e49521. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...749521S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049521. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3500307. PMID 23166696.


  28. ^ Jameson, Natalie M.; Hou, Zhuo-Cheng; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Weckle, Amy; Goodman, Morris; Steiper, Michael E.; Wildman, Derek E. (September 2011). "Genomic data reject the hypothesis of a prosimian primate clade". Journal of Human Evolution. 61 (3): 295–305. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.04.004. ISSN 0047-2484. PMID 21620437.


  29. ^ Pozzi, Luca; Hodgson, Jason A.; Burrell, Andrew S.; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Raaum, Ryan L.; Disotell, Todd R. (June 2014). "Primate phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes". Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 75: 165–183. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2014.02.023. ISSN 1055-7903. PMC 4059600. PMID 24583291.


  30. ^ Stanyon, Roscoe; Finstermeier, Knut; Zinner, Dietmar; Brameier, Markus; Meyer, Matthias; Kreuz, Eva; Hofreiter, Michael; Roos, Christian (16 July 2013). "A Mitogenomic Phylogeny of Living Primates". PLOS One. 8 (7): e69504. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...869504F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069504. ISSN 1932-6203.


  31. ^ Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Kirk, E. C. (2010). "New perspectives on anthropoid origins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (11): 4797–4804. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107. PMC 2841917. PMID 20212104.


  32. ^ Miller, E. R.; Gunnell, G. F.; Martin, R. D. (2005). "Deep Time and the Search for Anthropoid Origins" (PDF). American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 128: 60–95. doi:10.1002/ajpa.20352. PMID 16369958.


  33. ^ ab Chatterjee, Helen J; Ho, Simon Y.W.; Barnes, Ian; Groves, Colin (27 October 2009). "Estimating the phylogeny and divergence times of primates using a supermatrix approach". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 9 (1): 259. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-9-259. PMC 2774700. PMID 19860891.


  34. ^ O'Leary, M. A.; et al. (8 February 2013). "The placental mammal ancestor and the post–K-Pg radiation of placentals". Science. 339 (6120): 662–667. Bibcode:2013Sci...339..662O. doi:10.1126/science.1229237. PMID 23393258.


  35. ^ Lee, M. (September 1999). "Molecular Clock Calibrations and Metazoan Divergence Dates". Journal of Molecular Evolution. 49 (3): 385–391. Bibcode:1999JMolE..49..385L. doi:10.1007/PL00006562. PMID 10473780.


  36. ^ "Scientists Push Back Primate Origins From 65 Million To 85 Million Years Ago". Science Daily. Retrieved 2008-10-24.


  37. ^ Tavaré, S.; Marshall, C. R.; Will, O.; Soligo, C.; Martin R.D. (April 18, 2002). "Using the fossil record to estimate the age of the last common ancestor of extant primates". Nature. 416 (6882): 726–729. Bibcode:2002Natur.416..726T. doi:10.1038/416726a. PMID 11961552.


  38. ^ Klonisch, T.; Froehlich, C.; Tetens, F.; Fischer, B.; Hombach-Klonisch, S. (2001). "Molecular Remodeling of Members of the Relaxin Family During Primate Evolution". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 18 (3): 393–403. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a003815. PMID 11230540. Retrieved 2008-08-22.


  39. ^ abcde Horvath, J.; et al. (2008). "Development and Application of a Phylogenomic Toolkit: Resolving the Evolutionary History of Madagascar's Lemurs". Genome Research. 18 (3): 489–499. doi:10.1101/gr.7265208. PMC 2259113. PMID 18245770. Retrieved 2008-08-22.


  40. ^ abcd Sellers, Bill (2000-10-20). "Primate Evolution" (PDF). University of Edinburgh. pp. 13–17. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-10-29. Retrieved 2008-10-23.


  41. ^ abcd Hartwig, W. (2007). "Primate Evolution". In Campbell, C.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. pp. 13–17. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.


  42. ^ Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Christopher Kirk, E.; Ross, C. F. (2010). "Darwinius masillae is a strepsirrhine—a reply to Franzen et al. (2009)" (PDF). Journal of Human Evolution. 59 (5): 567–573, discussion 573–9. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.01.003. PMID 20188396. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-05-17.


  43. ^ Ciochon, R. & Fleagle, J. (1987). Primate Evolution and Human Origins. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. p. 72. ISBN 978-0-202-01175-2.


  44. ^ ab Garbutt, N. (2007). Mammals of Madagascar, A Complete Guide. A&C Black Publishers. pp. 85–86. ISBN 978-0-300-12550-4.


  45. ^ ab Mittermeier, R.A.; et al. (2006). Lemurs of Madagascar (2nd ed.). Conservation International. pp. 23–26. ISBN 1-881173-88-7.


  46. ^ Shekelle, M. (2005). Evolutionary Biology of Tarsiers. Archived from the original on 2008-09-07. Retrieved 2008-08-22.


  47. ^ Schmidt, T.; et al. (3 May 2005). "Rapid electrostatic evolution at the binding site for cytochrome c on cytochrome c oxidase in anthropoid primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (18): 6379–6384. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.6379S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0409714102. PMC 1088365. PMID 15851671.


  48. ^ Wade, Lizzie (June 5, 2013). "Early Primate Weighed Less Than an Ounce". ScienceNow. Archived from the original on 2013-06-08. Retrieved 2013-06-07.


  49. ^ Kay, R. F. (2012). "Evidence for an Asian origin of stem anthropoid s". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (26): 10132–10133. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910132K. doi:10.1073/pnas.1207933109. PMC 3387095. PMID 22699505.


  50. ^ Chaimanee, Y.; Chavasseau, O.; Beard, K. C.; Kyaw, A. A.; Soe, A. N.; Sein, C.; Lazzari, V.; Marivaux, L.; Marandat, B.; Swe, M.; Rugbumrung, M.; Lwin, T.; Valentin, X.; Zin-Maung-Maung-Thein; Jaeger, J. -J. (2012). "Late Middle Eocene primate from Myanmar and the initial anthropoid colonization of Africa". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (26): 10293–10297. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910293C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1200644109. PMC 3387043. PMID 22665790.


  51. ^ Marivaux, L.; et al. (2005-06-14). "Anthropoid primates from the Oligocene of Pakistan (Bugti Hills): Data on early anthropoid evolution and biogeography". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (24): 8436–8441. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8436M. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503469102. PMC 1150860. PMID 15937103.


  52. ^ Schrago, C.G. & Russo, C.A.M. (2003). "Timing the Origin of New World Monkeys" (PDF Reprint). Molecular Biology and Evolution. 20 (10): 1620–1625. doi:10.1093/molbev/msg172. PMID 12832653.


  53. ^ Houle, A. (1999). "The origin of platyrrhines: An evaluation of the Antarctic scenario and the floating island model". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 109 (4): 541–559. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199908)109:4<541::AID-AJPA9>3.0.CO;2-N. PMID 10423268.


  54. ^ Andrews, P. & Kelley, J. (2007). "Middle Miocene Dispersals of Apes". Folia Primatologica. 78 (5–6): 328–343. doi:10.1159/000105148. PMID 17855786.


  55. ^ abcdefghijklmnopqrstu Strier, K. (2007). Primate Behavioral Ecology (3rd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. pp. 7, 64, 71, 77, 182–185, 273–280, 284, 287–298. ISBN 0-205-44432-6.


  56. ^ Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Evolution and the Emergence of Humans". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. p. 650. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.


  57. ^ Tenaza, R. (1984). "Songs of hybrid gibbons (Hylobates lar × H. muelleri)". American Journal of Primatology. 8 (3): 249–253. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350080307.


  58. ^ ab Bernsteil, I. S. (1966). "Naturally occurring primate hybrid". Science. 154 (3756): 1559–1560. Bibcode:1966Sci...154.1559B. doi:10.1126/science.154.3756.1559. PMID 4958933.


  59. ^ Sugawara, K. (January 1979). "Sociological study of a wild group of hybrid baboons between Papio anubis and P. hamadryas in the Awash Valley, Ethiopia". Primates. 20 (1): 21–56. doi:10.1007/BF02373827.


  60. ^ Jolly, C. J.; Woolley-Barker, Tamsin; et al. (1997). "Intergeneric Hybrid Baboons". International Journal of Primatology. 18 (4): 597–627. doi:10.1023/A:1026367307470.


  61. ^ Liu, Zhen; et al. (24 January 2018). "Cloning of Macaque Monkeys by Somatic Cell Nuclear Transfer". Cell. 172: 881–887.e7. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.01.020. Retrieved 24 January 2018.


  62. ^ Normile, Dennis (24 January 2018). "These monkey twins are the first primate clones made by the method that developed Dolly". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aa1066. Retrieved 24 January 2018.


  63. ^ Cyranoski, David (24 January 2018). "First monkeys cloned with technique that made Dolly the sheep - Chinese scientists create cloned primates that could revolutionize studies of human disease". Nature. 553: 387–388. Bibcode:2018Natur.553..387C. doi:10.1038/d41586-018-01027-z. Retrieved 24 January 2018.


  64. ^ Briggs, Helen (24 January 2018). "First monkey clones created in Chinese laboratory". BBC News. Retrieved 24 January 2018.


  65. ^ Associated Press (24 January 2018). "Scientists Successfully Clone Monkeys; Are Humans Up Next?". The New York Times. Retrieved 24 January 2018.


  66. ^ abcd Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Characteristics of Primates". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. p. 630. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.


  67. ^ Campbell, B. G. & Loy, J. D. (2000). Humankind Emerging (8th edition). Allyn & Bacon. p. 85. ISBN 0-673-52364-0.


  68. ^ ab Aiello, L. & Dean, C. (1990). An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy. Academic Press. p. 193. ISBN 0-12-045590-0.


  69. ^ abcdef "Primate". Encyclopædia Britannica Online. Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc. 2008. Retrieved 2008-07-21.


  70. ^ abcdefg Myers, P. (1999). ""Primates" (On-line)". Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved 2008-06-03.


  71. ^ ab White, T. & Kazlev, A. (2006-01-08). "Archonta: Primates". Palaeos. Archived from the original on 2008-05-12. Retrieved 2008-06-03.


  72. ^ Garber PA, Rehg JA (November 1999). "The ecological role of the prehensile tail in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus)". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 110 (3): 325–39. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199911)110:3<325::AID-AJPA5>3.0.CO;2-D. PMID 10516564.


  73. ^ Russo GA, Young JW (November 2011). "Tail growth tracks the ontogeny of prehensile tail use in capuchin monkeys (Cebus albifrons and C. apella)". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 146 (3): 465–73. doi:10.1002/ajpa.21617. PMID 21953012.


  74. ^ Friderun Ankel-Simons (27 July 2010). Primate Anatomy: An Introduction. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-08-046911-9.


  75. ^ abcdefg Macdonald, David (2006). "Primates". The Encyclopedia of Mammals. The Brown Reference Group plc. pp. 290–307. ISBN 0-681-45659-0.


  76. ^ Ash, M. M.; Nelson, S. J.; Wheeler, R. C. (2003). Wheeler's Dental Anatomy, Physiology, and Occlusion. W.B. Saunders. p. 12. ISBN 978-0-7216-9382-8.


  77. ^ Arrese, C. A.; Oddy, Alison Y.; et al. (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 272 (1565): 791–6. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3009. PMC 1599861. PMID 15888411.


  78. ^ Bowmaker, J. K.; Astell, S.; Hunt, D. M.; Mollon, J. D. (1991). "Photosensitive and photostable pigments in the retinae of Old World monkeys" (pdf). The Journal of Experimental Biology. 156 (1): 1–19. ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 2051127. Retrieved 2008-06-16.


  79. ^ ab Surridge, A. K. & D. Osorio (2003). "Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 18 (4): 198–205. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00012-0.


  80. ^ Lucas, P. W.; Dominy, N. J.; Riba-Hernandez, P.; Stoner, K. E.; Yamashita, N.; Loría-Calderón, E.; Petersen-Pereira, W.; Rojas-Durán, Y.; Salas-Pena, R.; Solis-Madrigal, S.; Osorio, D.; Darvell, B. W. (2003). "Evolution and function of routine trichromatic vision in primates". Evolution. 57 (11): 2636–43. doi:10.1554/03-168. PMID 14686538.


  81. ^ abcde Sussman, R. W. (2003). Primate Ecology and Social Structure, Volume 2: New World Monkeys (Revised First ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 77–80, 132–133, 141–143. ISBN 0-536-74364-9.


  82. ^ abc Lindenfors, Patrik; Tullberg, Birgitta S. (1998). "Phylogenetic analyses of primate size evolution: the consequences of sexual selection". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 64 (4): 413–447. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1998.tb00342.x. ISSN 0024-4066.


  83. ^ Ralls, K. (1976). "Mammals in Which Females are Larger Than Males". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 51 (2): 245–76. doi:10.1086/409310. PMID 785524.


  84. ^ Lindstedtand & Boyce; Boyce, Mark S. (July 1985). "Seasonality, Fasting Endurance, and Body Size in Mammals". The American Naturalist. 125 (6): 873. doi:10.1086/284385.


  85. ^ Thorén, Sandra; Lindenfors, Patrik; Kappeler, Peter M. (2006). "Phylogenetic analyses of dimorphism in primates: Evidence for stronger selection on canine size than on body size". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 130 (1): 50–59. doi:10.1002/ajpa.20321. ISSN 0002-9483.


  86. ^ Frisch, J. E. (1963). "Sex-differences in the canines of the gibbon (Hylobates lar)". Primates. 4 (2): 1–10. doi:10.1007/BF01659148.


  87. ^ Kay, R. F. (1975). "The functional adaptations of primate molar teeth". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 43 (2): 195–215. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330430207. PMID 810034.


  88. ^ Crook, J. H. (1972). "Sexual selection, dimorphism, and social organization in the primates". In Campbell, B. G. Sexual selection and the descent of man. Aldine Transaction. p. 246. ISBN 978-0-202-02005-1.


  89. ^ ab Cheverud, J. M.; Dow, M. M.; Leutenegger, W. (November 1985). "The quantitative assessment of phylogenetic constraints in comparative analyses: Sexual dimorphism in body weight among primates". Evolution. 39 (6): 1335–1351. doi:10.2307/2408790. JSTOR 2408790.


  90. ^ Leutenegger, W.; Cheverud, J. M. (1982). "Correlates of sexual dimorphism in primates: Ecological and size variables". International Journal of Primatology. 3 (4): 387–402. doi:10.1007/BF02693740.


  91. ^ ab Plavcan, J. M. (2001). "Sexual dimorphism in primate evolution". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 33: 25–53. doi:10.1002/ajpa.10011. PMID 11786990.


  92. ^ O'Higgins, P.; Collard, M. (2002). "Sexual dimorphism and facial growth in papionine monkeys". Journal of Zoology. 257 (2): 255–72. doi:10.1017/S0952836902000857.


  93. ^ Sussman, R. W. (1999). Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 1: Lorises, Lemurs and Tarsiers. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 78, 89–90, 108, 121–123, 233. ISBN 0-536-02256-9.


  94. ^ Glazier, S. D.; Flowerday, C. A. (2003). Selected Readings in the Anthropology of Religion: Theoretical and Methodological Essays. Greenwood Publishing Group. p. 53. ISBN 9780313300905.


  95. ^ Kappeler, Peter; van Schaik, C. (2003). "Primate social systems".


  96. ^ Wrangham, R. W. (1982). "Mutualism, kinship and social evolution". Current Problems in Sociobiology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 269–89. ISBN 0-521-24203-7.


  97. ^ Neanderthal family found cannibalised in cave in Spain, BBC


  98. ^ Bowdler, Neil (2 June 2011). "Ancient cave women 'left childhood homes'". BBC News. Retrieved 2011-06-02.


  99. ^ Copeland SR, et al. (2011). "Strontium isotope evidence for landscape use by early hominins". Nature. 474: 76–78. doi:10.1038/nature10149. PMID 21637256.


  100. ^ Fiore, A. D. & Campbell, C. J. (2007). "The Atelines". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 175. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.


  101. ^ Bartlett, T. Q. (2007). "The Hylobatidae". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. K. Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 283. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.


  102. ^ Knott, C. D. & Kahlenberg, S. M. (2007). "Orangutans in Perspective". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 294. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.


  103. ^ ab Charpentier MJ, Widdig A, Alberts SC (December 2007). "Inbreeding depression in non-human primates: a historical review of methods used and empirical data". American Journal of Primatology. 69 (12): 1370–86. doi:10.1002/ajp.20445. PMID 17486606.


  104. ^ Ralls K, Ballou J (1982). "Effect of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates" (PDF). International Journal of Primatology. 3 (4): 491–505. doi:10.1007/BF02693747.


  105. ^ Constable, J. L.; Ashley, M. V.; Goodall, J.; Pusey, A. E. (May 2001). "Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees". Molecular Ecology. 10 (5): 1279–300. doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2001.01262.x. PMID 11380884.


  106. ^ abcde Rowe, N. (1996). The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates. Pogonias Press. pp. 4, 139, 143, 154, 185, 223. ISBN 0-9648825-0-7.


  107. ^ ab Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Societies". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 621–623. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.


  108. ^ Smuts, B.B., Cheney, D.L. Seyfarth, R.M., Wrangham, R.W., & Struhsaker, T.T. (Eds.) (1987). Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press for articles on the structure and function of various primate societies.


  109. ^ Shultz, S. & Thomsett, S. (2007). "Interactions between African Crowned Eagles and Their Prey Community". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K. & Noe, R. Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. p. 181. ISBN 0-521-81633-5.


  110. ^ ab Bshary, R. (2007). "Interactions between Red Colobus Monkeys and Chimpanzees". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Noe, R. Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. pp. 155–170. ISBN 0-521-81633-5.


  111. ^ Stanford, C. (1998). Chimpanzee and Red Colobus : the ecology of predator and prey. Harvard University Press. pp. 130–138, 233. ISBN 0-674-00722-0.


  112. ^ ab Boinski, S. (2000). "Social Manipulation Within and Between Troops Mediates Primate Group Movement". In Boinski, S.; Garber, P. On the Move : how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press. pp. 447–448. ISBN 0-226-06340-2.


  113. ^ Boesch, C.; Boesch, H. (1990). "Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees". Folia Primatologica. 54 (1–2): 86–99. doi:10.1159/000156428. PMID 2157651.


  114. ^ Westergaard, G. C.; Lundquist, A. L.; et al. (1998). "Why some capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use probing tools (and others do not)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 207–211. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.207. PMID 9642788.


  115. ^ de Waal, F. B. M.; Davis, J. M. (2003). "Capuchin cognitive ecology: cooperation based on projected returns". Neuropsychologia. 41 (2): 221–228. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00152-5. PMID 12459220.


  116. ^ Paar, L. A.; Winslow, J. T.; Hopkins, W. D.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). "Recognizing facial cues: Individual discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 114 (1): 47–60. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.47. PMC 2018744. PMID 10739311.


  117. ^ Paar, L. A.; de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). "Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees". Nature. 399 (6737): 647–648. Bibcode:1999Natur.399..647P. doi:10.1038/21345. PMID 10385114.


  118. ^ Fujita, K.; Watanabe, K.; Widarto, T. H.; Suryobroto, B. (1997). "Discrimination of macaques by macaques: The case of sulawesi species". Primates. 38 (3): 233–245. doi:10.1007/BF02381612.


  119. ^ Call, J. (2001). "Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 115 (2): 159–171. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.159. PMID 11459163.


  120. ^ Itakura, S.; Tanaka, M. (June 1998). "Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 119–126. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.119. PMID 9642782.


  121. ^ Gouteux, S.; Thinus-Blanc, C.; Vauclair, J. (2001). "Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 130 (3): 505–519. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.505.


  122. ^ Tomasello, M. & Call, J. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press US. ISBN 978-0-19-510624-4.


  123. ^ abc Deaner, R. O.; van Schaik, C. P.; Johnson, V. E. (2006). "Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A metaanalysis of nonhuman primate studies" (PDF). Evolutionary Psychology. 4: 149–196. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-28. Retrieved 2011-07-04.


  124. ^ abc Reader, S. M.; Hager, Y.; Laland, K. N. (2011). "The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence" (PDF). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 366 (1567): 1017–1027. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0342. PMC 3049098. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-10-03. Retrieved 2011-07-04.


  125. ^ Liman, E. R.; Innan, H. (2003). "Relaxed selective pressure on an essential component of pheromone transduction in primate evolution" (pdf). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 100 (6): 3328–3332. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.3328L. doi:10.1073/pnas.0636123100. PMC 152292. PMID 12631698. Retrieved 2008-07-23.


  126. ^ Egnor, R.; Miller, C.; Hauser, M.D. (2004). "Nonhuman Primate Communication" (PDF). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (2nd ed.). Elsevier. ISBN 0-08-044299-4. Archived from the original (pdf) on 2008-09-10.


  127. ^ Burrows, A. M. (2008). "The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance" (PDF). BioEssays. 30 (3): 212–225. doi:10.1002/bies.20719.


  128. ^ Pollick, A. S.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). "Ape gestures and language evolution". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (19): 8184–8189. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.8184P. doi:10.1073/pnas.0702624104. PMC 1876592.


  129. ^ Ramsier, M.A.; Cunningham, A.J.; Moritz, G.L.; Finneran, J.J.; Williams, C.V.; Ong, P.S.; Gursky-Doyen, S.L.; Dominy, N.J. (2012). "Primate communication in the pure ultrasound". Biology Letters. 8 (4): 508–511. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.1149. PMC 3391437. PMID 22319094.


  130. ^ "Black howler monkey". Smithsonian’s National Zoo & Conservation Biology Institute. Retrieved 2016-07-10.


  131. ^ Kelemen, G.; Sade, J. (1960). "The vocal organ of the Howling monkey (Alouatta palliata)". Journal of Morphology. 107 (2): 123–140. doi:10.1002/jmor.1051070202.


  132. ^ da Cunha, R. G. T.; Byrne, R. (2006). "Roars of Black Howler Monkeys (Alouatta caraya): Evidence for a Function in Inter-Group Spacing". Behaviour. 143 (10): 1169–1199. doi:10.1163/156853906778691568. JSTOR 4536401.


  133. ^ Seyfarth, R. M.; Cheney, D. L.; Marler, Peter (1980). "Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic communication in a Free-Ranging Primate" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 28 (4): 1070–1094. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2.


  134. ^ Fitch, W. T.; de Boer, B.; Mathur, N.; Ghazanfar, A. A. (2016). "Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready". Science Advances. 2 (12): e1600723. Bibcode:2016SciA....2E0723F. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1600723.


  135. ^ Boë L.-J.; Berthommier, F.; Legou, T.; Captier, G.; Kemp, C.; Sawallis, T. R.; et al. (2017). "Evidence of a Vocalic Proto-System in the Baboon (Papio papio) Suggests Pre-Hominin Speech Precursors". PLOS One. 12 (1): e0169321. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1269321B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169321.


  136. ^ "Psychopathology in great apes: Concepts, treatment options and possible homologies to human psychiatric disorders". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (8): 1246–1259. 2006-01-01. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.09.002. ISSN 0149-7634.


  137. ^ "Behavioural, hormonal and neurobiological mechanisms of aggressive behaviour in human and nonhuman primates". Physiology & Behavior. 143: 121–135. 2015-05-01. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.053. ISSN 0031-9384.


  138. ^ Wrangham, Richard W. (2018-01-09). "Two types of aggression in human evolution". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (2): 245–253. doi:10.1073/pnas.1713611115. ISSN 1091-6490. PMID 29279379.


  139. ^ Opie, Christopher; Atkinson, Quentin D.; Dunbarc, Robin I. M.; Shultz, Susanne (2013). "Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 110 (33): 13328–13332. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11013328O. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307903110. PMC 3746880. PMID 23898180.


  140. ^ De Ruiter, Jan R.; Van Hooff, Jan A. R. A. M. & Scheffrahn, Wolfgang (1994). "Social and genetic aspects of paternity in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis)". Behaviour. 129 (3–4): 203–24. doi:10.1163/156853994x00613. JSTOR 4535195.


  141. ^ Kappeler, Peter M. (1998). "Nests, Tree Holes, and the Evolution of Primate Life Histories". American Journal of Primatology. 46 (1): 7–33. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1998)46:1<7::AID-AJP3>3.0.CO;2-#. PMID 9730211.


  142. ^ Ross, Caroline (1991). "Park or ride? Evolution of infant carrying in primates". International Journal of Primatology. Kluwer Academic Publishing. 22 (5): 749–771. doi:10.1023/A:1012065332758.


  143. ^ Mintz, Zoe (14 January 2014). "Humans And Primates Burn 50 Percent Fewer Calories Each Day Than Other Mammals". www.ibtimes.com. IBT Media Inc. Retrieved 2014-01-14.


  144. ^ Walker ML, Herndon JG; Herndon (2008). "Menopause in nonhuman primates?". Biology of Reproduction. 79 (3): 398–406. doi:10.1095/biolreprod.108.068536. PMC 2553520. PMID 18495681.


  145. ^ Milton, K. (1993). "Diet and Primate Evolution" (PDF). Scientific American. Vol. 269 no. 2. pp. 86–93. Bibcode:1993SciAm.269b..86M. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0893-86. PMID 8351513.


  146. ^ Pollock, J. I.; Mullin, R. J. (1986). "Vitamin C biosynthesis in prosimians: Evidence for the anthropoid affinity of Tarsius". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 73 (1): 65–70. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330730106. PMID 3113259.


  147. ^ Milliken, G. W.; Ward, J. P.; Erickson, C. J. (1991). "Independent digit control in foraging by the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis)". Folia Primatologica. 56 (4): 219–224. doi:10.1159/000156551. PMID 1937286.


  148. ^ Hiller, C. (2000). "Theropithecus gelada". Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved 2008-08-08.


  149. ^ Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. (2003). "Introduction". In Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. Tarsiers Past, Present and Future. Rutgers University Press. p. 1. ISBN 0-8135-3236-1.


  150. ^ Goodall, Jane (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. ISBN 0-674-11649-6.


  151. ^ Guernsey, Paul. "WHAT DO CHIMPS EAT?". All About Wildlife. Retrieved 2013-04-22.


  152. ^ Ihobe H (1992). "Observations on the meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire". Primates. 33 (2): 247–250. doi:10.1007/BF02382754.


  153. ^ Rafert, J. and E.O. Vineberg (1997). "Bonobo Nutrition – relation of captive diet to wild diet, Archived 2012-04-25 at the Wayback Machine" Bonobo Husbandry Manual, American Association of Zoos and Aquariums


  154. ^ Surbeck, M; Fowler, A; Deimel, C; Hohmann, G (2008). "Evidence for the consumption of arboreal, diurnal primates by bonobos (Pan paniscus)". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (2): 171–4. doi:10.1002/ajp.20634. PMID 19058132.


  155. ^ Surbeck M, Hohmann G; Hohmann (14 October 2008). "Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park". Current Biology. 18 (19): R906–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.040. PMID 18957233.


  156. ^ Fichtel, Claudia (2012). "Predation". In Mitani, John C.; Call, Josep; Kappeler, Peter M.; Palombit, Ryne A.; Silk, Joan B. The Evolution of Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press. pp. 169–84. ISBN 0-226-53172-4.


  157. ^ ab "Tool use, hunting & other discoveries". The Jane Goodall Institute. Retrieved 2013-08-01.


  158. ^ "Bonobos". ApeTag. 2010. Archived from the original on 2013-11-02. Retrieved 2013-08-03.


  159. ^
    Gruber, T.; Clay, Z.; Zuberbühler, K. (2010). "A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 80 (6): 1023–1033. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.005.



  160. ^ Bower, B. (18 April 2011). "Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish". Wired. Retrieved 2013-08-05.


  161. ^ Breuer, T.; Ndoundou-Hockemba, M.; Fishlock, V. (2005). "First observation of tool use in wild gorillas". PLOS Biology. 3 (11): e380. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030380. PMC 1236726. PMID 16187795.


  162. ^ Fragaszy, D.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E.; Ottoni, E.B.; de Oliveira, M.G. (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools". American Journal of Primatology. 64 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1002/ajp.20085. PMID 15580579.


  163. ^ Gumert, M.D.; Kluck, M.; Malaivijitnond, S. (2009). "The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (7): 594–608. doi:10.1002/ajp.20694. PMID 19405083.


  164. ^ Hamilton, W.J.; Buskirk, R.E.; Buskirk, W.H. (1975). "Defensive stoning by baboons". Nature. 256 (5517): 488–489. Bibcode:1975Natur.256..488H. doi:10.1038/256488a0.


  165. ^ Fichtel, C.; Kappeler, P. M. (2010). "Chapter 19: Human universals and primate symplesiomorphies: Establishing the lemur baseline". In Kappeler, P. M.; Silk, J. B. Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals. Springer. ISBN 978-3-642-02724-6.


  166. ^ "Sumatran orangutans". OrangutanIslands.com. Archived from the original on 2013-11-26. Retrieved 2013-08-02.


  167. ^ van Schaik, C.; Fox, E.; Sitompul, A. (1996). "Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans". Naturwissenschaften. 83 (4): 186–188. Bibcode:1996NW.....83..186V. doi:10.1007/BF01143062. PMID 8643126.


  168. ^ Gill, Victoria (22 July 2011). "Mandrill monkey makes 'pedicuring' tool". BBC. Retrieved 2013-08-11.


  169. ^ Vancatova, M. (2008). "Gorillas and Tools – Part I". Retrieved 2013-08-04.


  170. ^ Reed, K.; Fleagle, J. (August 15, 1995). "Geographic and climatic control of primate diversity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 92 (17): 7874–7876. Bibcode:1995PNAS...92.7874R. doi:10.1073/pnas.92.17.7874. PMC 41248. PMID 7644506.


  171. ^ Chapman, C. & Russo, S. (2007). "Primate Seed Dispersal". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 510. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.


  172. ^ Long, Y. C.; Kirkpatrick, R. C.; Zhong, T.; Xiao, L. (April 1994). "Report on the distribution, population, and ecology of the Yunnan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus bieti)". Primates. 35 (2): 241–250. doi:10.1007/BF02382060.


  173. ^ Schaller, G. B. (1963). The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago: University Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-73635-8.


  174. ^ Wolfe, L. D. & Fuentes, A. (2007). "Ethnoprimatology". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 692. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.


  175. ^ Renquist, D. M. & Whitney, R. A. (1987). "Zoonoses Acquired from Pet Primates". Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice. 17 (1): 219–240. doi:10.1016/s0195-5616(87)50614-3. Retrieved 2008-08-11.


  176. ^ "The Universal Declaration of Human Rights". United Nations. 1948. Archived from the original on 2014-12-08. Retrieved 2008-12-02.


  177. ^ ab Cavalieri, P. & Singer, P. "Declaration on Great Apes". Great Ape Project. Archived from the original on 2008-08-20. Retrieved 2008-06-16.


  178. ^ Glendinning, L. (26 June 2008). "Spanish parliament approves 'human rights' for apes". The Guardian. Retrieved 2008-11-10.


  179. ^ Singer, P. (18 July 2008). "Of great apes and men". The Guardian. Retrieved 2008-11-10.


  180. ^ Mott, M. (16 September 2003). "The Perils of Keeping Monkeys as Pets". National Geographic. Archived from the original on 2013-06-08. Retrieved 2013-02-06.


  181. ^ ab Workman, C. (June 2004). "Primate conservation in Vietnam: toward a holistic environmental narrative". American Anthropologist. 106 (2): 346–352. doi:10.1525/aa.2004.106.2.346.


  182. ^ "IPPL News: The US Pet Monkey Trade". International Primate Protection League. 2003. Archived from the original on 2008-07-25. Retrieved 2008-08-04.


  183. ^ Bushnell, D. (1958). "The Beginnings of Research in Space Biology at the Air Force Missile Development Center, 1946–1952". History of Research in Space Biology and Biodynamics. NASA. Retrieved 2008-08-18.


  184. ^ Blumenthal, D. (1987-06-17). "Monkeys as Helpers To Quadriplegics At Home". The New York Times. Retrieved 2008-10-08.


  185. ^ Newman, James L. (2013). Encountering Gorillas: A Chronicle of Discovery, Exploitation, Understanding, and Survival. Plymouth, United Kingdom: Rowman and Littlefield. p. 173. ISBN 978-1-4422-1957-1.


  186. ^ ab "The supply and use of primates in the EU". European Biomedical Research Association. 1996. Archived from the original on 2012-01-17. Retrieved 2008-08-18.


  187. ^ Chen, F. C.; Li, W. H. (February 2001). "Genomic divergences between humans and other hominoids and the effective population size of the common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees". American Journal of Human Genetics. 68 (2): 444–456. doi:10.1086/318206. PMC 1235277. PMID 11170892.


  188. ^ Conlee, K. M.; Hoffeld, E. H.; Stephens, M. L. (2004). "A Demographic Analysis of Primate Research in the United States" (PDF). Alternatives to Laboratory Animals. 32 (Sup 1): 315–322. Archived from the original (pdf) on 2008-06-25. Retrieved 2008-10-08.


  189. ^ Presented to Parliament by the Secretary of State for the Home Department by Command of Her Majesty (July 2006). Statistics of scientific procedures on living animals: Great Britain 2005 (pdf). The Stationery Office. ISBN 0-10-168772-9. Retrieved 2008-06-16.


  190. ^ "Nonhuman Primates: Research Animals". Animal Welfare Information Center. United States Department of Agriculture. Archived from the original on 2008-10-03. Retrieved 2008-07-14.


  191. ^ "Directive 86/609". European Coalition to End Animal Experiments. Archived from the original on 2008-09-29. Retrieved 2008-10-08.


  192. ^ ab Estrada, Alejandro; Garber, Paul A.; Rylands, Anthony B.; Roos, Christian; Fernandez-Duque, Eduardo; Fiore, Anthony Di; Nekaris, K. Anne-Isola; Nijman, Vincent; Heymann, Eckhard W. (2017-01-01). "Impending extinction crisis of the world's primates: Why primates matter". Science Advances. 3 (1): e1600946. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1600946. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 5242557. PMID 28116351.


  193. ^ IFAW (2005). Born to be wild: Primates are not pets (PDF). International Fund for Animal Welfare. Archived from the original (pdf) on 2011-02-18. Retrieved 2011-02-26.


  194. ^ CITES (2010-10-14). "Appendices I, II and III". Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora. Retrieved 2012-04-02.


  195. ^ Grubb, P. (1998). "The Sierra Leone monkey drives". Mammals of Ghana, Sierra Leone, and the Gambia. St. Ives: Trendrine. pp. 214–219. ISBN 0-9512562-4-6.


  196. ^ ab Chapman, C. A.; Peres, C. A. (2001). "Primate conservation in the new millennium: the role of scientists". Evolutionary Anthropology. 10 (1): 16–33. doi:10.1002/1520-6505(2001)10:1<16::AID-EVAN1010>3.0.CO;2-O.


  197. ^ ab Mittermeier, R. A. & Cheney, D. L. (1987). "Conservation of primates and their habitats". In Smuts, B. B.; Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R. M.; Wrangham, R. W. & Struhsaker, T. T. Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 477–490.


  198. ^ ab Southwick, C. H. & Siddiqi, M. F. (2001). "Status, conservation and management of primates in India" (PDF). Envis Bulletin: Wildlife and Protected Areas. 1 (1): 81–91. Archived from the original (pdf) on 2008-10-01. Retrieved 2008-08-04.


  199. ^ abcd Cowlishaw, G. & Dunbar, R. (2000). Primate Conservation Biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-11637-2.


  200. ^ Van Schaik, C. P.; Monk, K. A.; Robertson, J. M. Y. (2001). "Dramatic decline in orangutan numbers in the Leuser Ecosystem, northern Sumatra". Oryx. 35 (1): 14–25. doi:10.1046/j.1365-3008.2001.00150.x.


  201. ^ Purvis, A.; Gittleman, J. L.; Cowlishaw, G.; Mace, G. M. (2000). "Predicting extinction risk in declining species". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 267 (1456): 1947–1952. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1234. PMC 1690772. PMID 11075706.


  202. ^ abc Fa, J. E.; Juste, J.; Perez de Val, J.; Castroviejo, J. (1995). "Impact of market hunting on mammal species in Equatorial Guinea". Conservation Biology. 9 (5): 1107–1115. doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.1995.9051107.x.


  203. ^ Hill, C. M. (1997). "Crop-raiding by wild vertebrates: The farmer's perspective in an agricultural community in western Uganda". International Journal of Pest Management. 43 (1): 77–84. doi:10.1080/096708797229022.


  204. ^ Hill, C. M. (2002). "Primate conservation and local communities: Ethical issues and debates". American Anthropologist. 104 (4): 1184–1194. doi:10.1525/aa.2002.104.4.1184.


  205. ^ Choudhury, A. (2001). "Primates in Northeast India: an overview of their distribution and conservation status" (PDF). Envis Bulletin: Wildlife and Protected Areas. 1 (1): 92–101. Archived from the original (pdf) on 2008-10-01. Retrieved 2008-08-04.


  206. ^ Kumara, H. N.; Singh, M. (October 2004). "Distribution and abundance of primates in rainforests of the Western Ghats, Karnataka, India and the conservation of Macaca silenus". International Journal of Primatology. 25 (5): 1001–1018. doi:10.1023/B:IJOP.0000043348.06255.7f.


  207. ^ Nijman, V. (2004). "Conservation of the Javan gibbon Hylobates moloch: population estimates, local extinction, and conservation priorities" (PDF). The Raffles Bulletin of Zoology. 52 (1): 271–280. Archived from the original (pdf) on 2008-09-10. Retrieved 2008-08-04.


  208. ^ O'Brien, T. G.; Kinnaird, M. F.; Nurcahyo, A.; Iqbal, M.; Rusmanto, M. (April 2004). "Abundance and distribution of sympatric gibbons in a threatened Sumatran rain forest". International Journal of Primatology. 25 (2): 267–284. doi:10.1023/B:IJOP.0000019152.83883.1c.


  209. ^ Estrada, A.; Coates-Estrada, R.; Meritt, D. (September 1994). "Non-flying mammals and landscape changes in the tropical forest region of Los Tuxtlas, Mexico". Ecography. 17 (3): 229–241. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0587.1994.tb00098.x.


  210. ^ Marsh, L. K. (2003). "The nature of fragmentation.". In Marsh, L. K. Primates in Fragments: Ecology and Conservation. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. pp. 1–10. ISBN 0-306-47696-7.


  211. ^ Turner, I. M. (1996). "Species loss in fragments of tropical rain forest: a review of the evidence". Journal of Applied Ecology. 33 (2): 200–209. doi:10.2307/2404743. JSTOR 2404743.


  212. ^ Chiarello, A.G. (2003). "Primates of the Brazilian Atlantic forest: the influence of forest fragmentation on survival". In Marsh, L. K. Primates in Fragments: Ecology and Conservation. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. pp. 99–121. ISBN 978-0-306-47696-9.


  213. ^ Pope, T.R. (1996). "Socioecology, population fragmentation, and patterns of genetic loss in endangered primates". In Avise, J.; Hamrick, J. Conservation Genetics: Case Histories from Nature. Norwell: Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 119–159. ISBN 978-0-412-05581-2.


  214. ^ Mittermeier, R.A.; Wallis, J.; Rylands, A.B.; Ganzhorn, J.U.; Oates, J.F.; Williamson, E.A.; Palacios, E.; Heymann, E.W.; Kierulff, M.C.M.; Yongcheng, L.; Supriatna, J.; Roos, C.; Walker, S.; Cortés-Ortiz, L.; Schwitzer, C., eds. (2009). "Primates in Peril: The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates 2008–2010" (PDF). Illustrated by S.D. Nash. Arlington, VA.: IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), and Conservation International (CI): 23–26. ISBN 978-1-934151-34-1.


  215. ^ Oates, J. F.; Abedi-Lartey, M.; McGraw, W. S.; Struhsaker, T. T.; Whitesides, G. H. (October 2000). "Extinction of a West African Red Colobus Monkey". Conservation Biology. 14 (5): 1526–1532. doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.2000.99230.x.


  216. ^ McGraw, W. S. (June 2005). "Update on the Search for Miss Waldron's Red Colobus Monkey". International Journal of Primatology. 26 (3): 605–619. doi:10.1007/s10764-005-4368-9.



Literature cited



  • Benton, Michael J. (2005). "Chapter 3: Primate evolution". Vertebrate palaeontology. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-632-05637-8. Retrieved 2011-07-10.


  • Cartmill, M. (2010). "Primate Classification and Diversity". In Platt, M.; Ghazanfar, A. Primate Neuroethology. Oxford University Press. pp. 10–30. ISBN 978-0-19-532659-8.


  • Hartwig, W. (2011). "Chapter 3: Primate evolution". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Bearder, S. K.; Stumpf, R. M. Primates in Perspective (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. pp. 19–31. ISBN 978-0-19-539043-8.


  • Szalay, F.S.; Delson, E. (1980). Evolutionary History of the Primates. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0126801507. OCLC 893740473.


Further reading




  • David J. Chivers; Bernard A. Wood; Alan Bilsborough, eds. (1984). Food Acquisition and Processing in Primates. New York & London: Plenum Press. ISBN 0-306-41701-4.


External links



  • Primate at Curlie

  • Primate Info Net


  • Primates at Animal Diversity Web


  • Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University

  • High-Resolution Cytoarchitectural Primate Brain Atlases

  • EUPRIM-Net: European Primate Network

  • PrimateImages: Natural History Collection

  • Tree of Life web project













這個網誌中的熱門文章

How to read a connectionString WITH PROVIDER in .NET Core?

Node.js Script on GitHub Pages or Amazon S3

Museum of Modern and Contemporary Art of Trento and Rovereto